1. Briggs, G. E., & Naylor, J. C. (1962). The relative efficiency of several training methods as a function of transfer task complexity. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 64(5), 505.
2. Calmels, C., Holmes, P., Lopez, E., & Naman, V. (2006). Chronometric comparison of actual and imaged complex movement patterns. Journal of Motor Behavior, 38(5), 339-348.
3. Cerritelli, B., Maruff, P., Wilson, P., & Currie, J. (2000). The effect of an external load on the force and timing components of mentally represented actions. Behavioural brain research, 108(1), 91-96.
4. Coelho, C. J., Nusbaum, H. C., Rosenbaum, D. A., & Fenn, K. M. (2012). Imagined actions aren't just weak actions: Task variability promotes skill learning in physical practice but not in mental practice. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 38(6), 1759.
5. Dana, A., Shams, A., Allafan, N. Bahrami, AR. (2021). The relationship between attention and static balance disturbance in patients with Parkinson’s disease. Neurol Sci, 42, 5107–5115.
6. Debarnot, U., Maley, L., De Rossi, D., & Guillot, A. (2010). Motor interference does not impair the memory consolidation of imagined movements. Brain and cognition, 74(1), 52-57.
7. Decety, J., & Grèzes, J. (1999). Neural mechanisms subserving the perception of human actions. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 3(5), 172-178.
8. Decety, J., & Jeannerod, M. (1995). Mentally simulated movements in virtual reality: does Fitt's law hold in motor imagery? Behavioural brain research, 72(1-2), 127-134.
9. Driskell, J. E., Copper, C., & Moran, A. (1994). Does mental practice enhance performance? Journal of applied psychology, 79(4), 481.
10. Eagles, J. S., Carlsen, A. N., & MacKinnon, C. D. (2015). Neural processes mediating the preparation and release of focal motor output are suppressed or absent during imagined movement. Experimental Brain Research, 233(5), 1625-1637.
11. Fontana, F. E., Furtado Jr, O., Mazzardo, O., & Gallagher, J. D. (2009). Whole and part practice: a meta-analysis. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 109(2), 517-530.
12. Guadagnoli, M. A., & Lee, T. D. (2004). Challenge point: a framework for conceptualizing the effects of various practice conditions in motor learning. Journal of Motor Behavior, 36(2), 212-22.
13. Guillot, A., Di Rienzo, F., MacIntyre, T., Moran, A., & Collet, C. (2012). Imagining is not doing but involves specific motor commands: a review of experimental data related to motor inhibition. Frontiers in human neuroscience, 6(1), 247.
14. Guillot, A., Lebon, F., Rouffet, D., Champely, S., Doyon, J., & Collet, C. (2007). Muscular responses during motor imagery as a function of muscle contraction types. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 66(1), 18-27.
15. Hodges, N. J., Ong, N. T., Larssen, B. C., & Lim, S. B. (2011). What Observation of Motor Skills Does and Does Not Teach Us. Paper presented at the BIO Web of Conferences.
16. Jeannerod, M. (2001). Neural simulation of action: a unifying mechanism for motor cognition. Neuroimage, 14(11), 101-109.
17. Kunz, B. R., Creem-Regehr, S. H., & Thompson, W. B. (2009). Evidence for motor simulation in imagined locomotion. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 35(5), 1458.
18. MacIntyre, T. E., Madan, C. R., Moran, A. P., Collet, C., & Guillot, A. (2018). Motor imagery, performance and motor rehabilitation. Progress In Brain Research, 240, 141-159.
19. McCormick, S. A., Causer, J., & Holmes, P. S. (2013). Active vision during action execution, observation and imagery: evidence for shared motor representations. PLoS One, 8(6), e67761.
20. McDermott, P., Carolan, T., Gacy, M., Fisher, A., & Gronowski, M. (2012). Cost Effective Approaches to Training Reconnaissance Tasks on Unmanned Systems. American Institute of Aeronautics and Astronautics, 12(4), 18-25.
21. Munzert, J., Blischke, K., & Krüger, B. (2015). Motor imagery of locomotion with an additional load: actual load experience does not affect differences between physical and mental durations. Experimental brain research, 233(3), 809-816.
22. Naito, E., Kochiyama, T., Kitada, R., Nakamura, S., Matsumura, M., Yonekura, Y., & Sadato, N. (2002). Internally simulated movement sensations during motor imagery activate cortical motor areas and the cerebellum. Journal of Neuroscience, 22(9), 3683-3691.
23. O’Shea, H., & Moran, A. (2016). Chronometric and pupil-size measurements illuminate the relationship between motor execution and motor imagery in expert pianists. Psychology of Music, 44(6), 1289-1303.
24. Ong, N. T., Larssen, B. C., & Hodges, N. J. (2012). In the absence of physical practice, observation and imagery do not result in updating of internal models for aiming. Experimental brain research, 218(1), 9-19.
25. Reed, C. L. (2002). Chronometric comparisons of imagery to action: Visualizing versus physically performing springboard dives. Memory & Cognition, 30(8), 1169-1178.
26. Schöllhorn, W., Oelenberg, M., & Michelbrink, M. (2007). Can mental training enhance the learning effect after differencial training? A Tennis Serve Task In: Theodorakis Y, Goudas M, Papaioannou A, editors. Paper presented at the Proceedings of the 12th European Congress of Sport Psychology. Halkidiki: FEPSAC.
27. Shams, A., VaezMousavi, M., Aghdashipour, N. (2021). Psychometric Properties of Persian Version of the Movement Imagery Questionnaire for Children (MIQ-C). Sports Psychology, (1), 29-48.
28. Toussaint, L., & Blandin, Y. (2010). On the role of imagery modalities on motor learning. Journal of Sports Sciences, 28(5), 497-504.
29. VaezMousavi M, Shams A. (2017). Mental Health of Iranian Elite Athletes. Iran J Health Educ Health Promot. 5 (3) :191-202.